Blute Blog

Blute's blog about evolutionary theory: biological, sociocultural and gene-culture.

Posts Tagged ‘gene-culture coevolution

Entanglements: Ecology & evolution, genes & culture, us & others

leave a comment »

I was invited by Chris Kortright and Jaime Yard to take part in a session at the annual American Anthropological Association here meeting held last week in Montreal, a session which was titled “The Conceptual Work of ‘Ecology’”. The word that stuck out for me in the abstract provided for the session was “entanglements”. So I gave my talk the title above and here is the abstract of what I talked about.

“Not long ago it was thought that causal relationships between ecology and evolution were unidirectional – ecological environments (including physical environments and other species) structure evolving populations. We now know that the unidirectional view was false. Evolving populations also construct ecological environments (commonly called niche construction).

Similarly it was thought that causal relationships between genes and culture were unidirectional. The sociobiological/human behavioural ecological/evolutionary psychological revolution(s) revealed how the propensities of human nature, including our cultural nature, had been shaped by genetic evolution. But we now know that unidirectional view to have also been false. Anthropologists like William H. Durham played a significant role in showing how much our genetic evolution has been shaped by culture. A count (by Laland et. al. blogged about here on Dec. 8, 2010) includes 8 categories, some including as many as 30 genes, whose evolution can plausibly be linked to culturally-transmitted selection pressures. Thus came the era of not only gene-culture but also of culture-gene coevolution.

However, this theory and research remains anthropocentric. Genes and culture in the human species are viewed as coevolving in interaction with each other – i.e. excluding other species. However, the evidence that has been accumulating for decades now showing the ubiquity of culture and its evolution in other species implies that we are on the cusp of yet another revolution – interspecific gene-culture and culture-gene coevolution. And this revolution will not only be about our culture shaping their genes, but also about their genes shaping our culture.”

Gene-culture coevolution and genomics

leave a comment »

Theorists have been talking about, modelling and providing anecdotal evidence for dual inheritance and gene-culture coevolution at least since the mid 1980’s (e.g. see Robert Boyd and Peter J. Richerson’s, Culture and the Evolutionary Process and William H. Durham’s Coevolution: Genes, Culture and Human Diversity respectively). A number of traits in particular populations have been plausibly linked to culture-driven genetic change including the effect of herding cultures on the level of expression of genes for lactose tolerance, of farming cultures on copy number expansion of genes for starch digestion, and of slash and burn agriculture on a gene conferring resistance to malaria but which can also result in sickle-cell disease for example. At a recent conference (the off-year meeting of the ISHPSSB), John Odling-Smee kindly drew my attention to two articles published this year which suggest that to bear greater scientific fruit, the multidisciplinary literature on gene-culture coevolution needs to be connected more closely with human population genomics (Laland et. al. gated, Richerson et. al. gated).

Human population genomics has already yielded a great deal of information about the relatedness and migration histories of various human populations. However, methods have also been developed for detecting recent (on the order of 100,000 years) strong positive selection on genes in human populations. Pickrell, in a blog post this year on Genomes Unzipped has simplified the explanation of these methods which are rich with acronyms in the literature. All identify alleles which have gone up in frequency unusually fast – either by identifying unusually large allele frequency differences between two populations or by finding young alleles at unusually high frequency within a population.

Some cautions are in order. First, the results of different statistical techniques used to infer this linkage disequilibrium can vary from identifying only hundreds to identifying thousands of genes subject to recent positive selection. A recent simulation study however has argued for the superiority of one of these (Huff et. al.). Secondly, culture could exert strong positive or negative selection pressures on genes to be sure, but with negative frequency-dependence among evolving cultural alternatives for example, it could also conceivably simply just leave alternative alleles in competition with each other (Blute gated). And last but not least, there is the question of the extent to which even recent strong selection necessarily implies a cultural cause.

Nevertheless, and this is the point of the post, Laland et. al. in their Table 2 (gated but reproduced below) have identified eight categories of genes from the literature, some including as many as 30 genes, whose function or phenotype is broadly known, and for which cultural selection pressures can reasonably be inferred.

As “next-generation” high-throughput sequencing technologies become available and cheaper (Metzker gated), although the major impetus for them is medical, they will inevitably ultimately be used to provide a denser set of sequences from more individuals in more populations yielding more information. In short, the traditional sociobiological story of genes-to-culture causal arrows by means of genes selecting among culturally transmitted alternatives is finally being supplemented on a large scale by a story of culture-to-genes causal arrows by means of culture selecting among genetically transmitted alternatives – all to the better of understanding both human unity and diversity.

  • Genes identified as having been subject to recent rapid selection and their inferred cultural selection pressures (Reproduced from Laland et. al. 2010)

Genes

Function or phenotype

Inferred cultural selection pressure

Refs

LCT, MAN2A1, SI, SLC27A4, PPARD, SLC25A20, NCOA1, LEPR, LEPR, ADAMTS19, ADAMTS20, APEH, PLAU, HDAC8, UBR1, USP26, SCP2, NKX2-2, AMY1, ADH, NPY1R, NPY5R

Digestion of milk and dairy products; metabolism of carbohydrates, starch, proteins, lipids and phosphates; alcohol metabolism

Dairy farming and milk usage; dietary preferences; alcohol consumption

6,7,16,41,63,102,118,144,145

Cytochrome P450 genes (CYP3A5, CYP2E1, CYP1A2 and CYP2D6)

Detoxification of plant secondary compounds

Domestication of plants

6,63,146,147

CD58, APOBEC3F, CD72, FCRL2, TSLP, RAG1, RAG2, CD226, IGJ, TJP1, VPS37C, CSF2, CCNT2, DEFB118, STAB1, SP1, ZAP70, BIRC6, CUGBP1, DLG3, HMGCR, STS, XRN2, ATRN, G6PD, TNFSF5, HbC, HbE, HbS, Duffy, α-globin

Immunity, pathogen response; resistance to malaria and other crowd diseases

Dispersal, agriculture, aggregation and subsequent exposure to new pathogens; farming

6,7,8,14,16,50,63,148,149

LEPR, PON1, RAPTOR, MAPK14, CD36, DSCR1, FABP2, SOD1, CETP, EGFR, NPPA, EPHX2, MAPK1, UCP3, LPA, MMRN1

Energy metabolism, hot or cold tolerance; heat-shock genes

Dispersal and subsequent exposure to novel climates

14,150

SLC24A5, SLC25A2, EDAR, EDA2R, SLC24A4, KITLG, TYR, 6p25.3, OCA2, MC1R, MYO5A, DTNBP1, TYRP1, RAB27A, MATP, MC2R, ATRN, TRPM1, SILV, KRTAPs, DCT

The externally visible phenotype (skin pigmentation, hair thickness, eye and hair colour, and freckles)

Dispersal and local adaptation and/or sexual selection

9,14,63,97,101,151

CDK5RAP2, CENPJ, GABRA4, PSEN1, SYT1, SLC6A4, SNTG1, GRM3, GRM1, GLRA2, OR4C13, OR2B6, RAPSN, ASPM, RNT1, SV2B, SKP1A, DAB1, APPBP2, APBA2, PCDH15, PHACTR1, ALG10, PREP, GPM6A, DGKI, ASPM, MCPH1, FOXP2

Nervous system, brain function and development; language skills and vocal learning

Complex cognition on which culture is reliant; social intelligence; language use and vocal learning

6,7,14,63,68,69,70,78,149

BMP3, BMPR2, BMP5, GDF5

Skeletal development

Dispersal and sexual selection

6,63

MYH16, ENAM

Jaw muscle fibres; tooth-enamel thickness

Invention of cooking; diet

80,113

Written by Marion Blute

December 8, 2010 at 2:56 am